物种新进化--蚂蚁

The big-headed ant (Pheidole megacephala) is considered one of the world's worst invasive ant species. As the name implies, its colonies include soldier ants with disproportionately large heads. Their giant, muscle-bound noggins power their biting parts, the mandibles, which they use to attack other ants and cut up prey. In a new study, researchers report that big-headed ant colonies produce larger soldiers when they encounter other ants that know how to fight back.

The new findings appear in the Biological Journal of the Linnean Society. Big-headed ants are world travelers, hitching rides with humans to get around. Scientists have found them in more than 1,600 sites across the globe (see map). Their arrival at a sufficiently warm destination (they cannot tolerate cold weather) spells almost certain doom for native ants, spiders, beetles and other invertebrates that are unaccustomed to their brand of warfare.

"If you think about the worst invasive species, ants frequently show up on those lists, and big-headed ants are among the most problematic," said University of Illinois entomology professor and animal biology department head Andrew Suarez, who led the new study with postdoctoral researcher Bill Wills. "They are very aggressive. And unlike a lot of native ants, they produce large numbers of queens, so they have incredibly high potential for reproduction."

Big-headed ants spread out, assembling multiple nests that cooperate on defense, reproduction, territorial expansion and food procurement, the researchers said. Soldiers and non-soldiers in a Pheidole megacephala colony are sisters, so genetic changes do not account for their differing morphology. Changes in nutrition during development are primarily responsible for their different shapes and sizes, Suarez said. "We know that you can feed larvae differentially and that will change hormone levels that will cause a completely different developmental pathway to turn on, so you get this giant head, you become a big-headed ant rather than a small worker," he said.

The team wanted to know if environmental cues, such as the presence of other aggressive ants, would cause the big-headed ant colonies to produce more, or bigger, soldiers. To study this, they looked at the ants in five locales: Australia, Florida, Hawaii, Mauritius (an island off the southeast coast of Africa) and South Africa.

"Hawaii is interesting because there are no native ants there, so a lot of the local insect and arthropod fauna are not necessarily adapted to dealing with social insects," Wills said. "But the native ant fauna in Australia is very dominant and diverse." The study sites in Florida, Mauritius and South Africa represent a kind of intermediate condition, where the big-headed ants encounter competitors that are not as fierce or diverse as the ants of Australia, he said.

The team found that big-headed soldiers and non-soldiers grew largest in Australia and smallest in Hawaii. On average, the Australian big-headed ant soldiers were three times more massive than their equivalents in Hawaii, Wills said. The ants at the other study sites were intermediate in size.

Genetic analysis showed that the ants from all the study sites were closely related, Suarez said, ruling out the possibility that size variation was the result of long-term evolutionary change.

"This shows that they were able to adapt to a new environment relatively quickly," Suarez said. "It's kind of exciting and scary that it can happen so quickly."

(In a separate study, Wills found that fire ants can begin to morph their body size within 60 days in a new environment.)

The big-headed ants produced bigger workers in response to external cues, but they did not generate a larger share of soldiers in any of the sites, Suarez said.

"We found that the ratios of soldiers to non-soldiers were relatively constant across our populations," he said

The researchers hypothesize that the ants use chemical signals called pheromones to determine when the nest has the right proportion of soldiers to other workers. If this turns out to be the case, scientists may be able to control the ants with chemical signals without disrupting the surrounding ecology, Suarez said.

The big-headed ant (Pheidole megacephala) is considered one of the world's worst invasive ant species. As the name implies, its colonies include soldier ants with disproportionately large heads. Their giant, muscle-bound noggins power their biting parts, the mandibles, which they use to attack other ants and cut up prey. In a new study, researchers report that big-headed ant colonies produce larger soldiers when they encounter other ants that know how to fight back.

The new findings appear in the Biological Journal of the Linnean Society. Big-headed ants are world travelers, hitching rides with humans to get around. Scientists have found them in more than 1,600 sites across the globe (see map). Their arrival at a sufficiently warm destination (they cannot tolerate cold weather) spells almost certain doom for native ants, spiders, beetles and other invertebrates that are unaccustomed to their brand of warfare.

"If you think about the worst invasive species, ants frequently show up on those lists, and big-headed ants are among the most problematic," said University of Illinois entomology professor and animal biology department head Andrew Suarez, who led the new study with postdoctoral researcher Bill Wills. "They are very aggressive. And unlike a lot of native ants, they produce large numbers of queens, so they have incredibly high potential for reproduction."

Big-headed ants spread out, assembling multiple nests that cooperate on defense, reproduction, territorial expansion and food procurement, the researchers said. Soldiers and non-soldiers in a Pheidole megacephala colony are sisters, so genetic changes do not account for their differing morphology. Changes in nutrition during development are primarily responsible for their different shapes and sizes, Suarez said. "We know that you can feed larvae differentially and that will change hormone levels that will cause a completely different developmental pathway to turn on, so you get this giant head, you become a big-headed ant rather than a small worker," he said.

The team wanted to know if environmental cues, such as the presence of other aggressive ants, would cause the big-headed ant colonies to produce more, or bigger, soldiers. To study this, they looked at the ants in five locales: Australia, Florida, Hawaii, Mauritius (an island off the southeast coast of Africa) and South Africa.

"Hawaii is interesting because there are no native ants there, so a lot of the local insect and arthropod fauna are not necessarily adapted to dealing with social insects," Wills said. "But the native ant fauna in Australia is very dominant and diverse." The study sites in Florida, Mauritius and South Africa represent a kind of intermediate condition, where the big-headed ants encounter competitors that are not as fierce or diverse as the ants of Australia, he said.

The team found that big-headed soldiers and non-soldiers grew largest in Australia and smallest in Hawaii. On average, the Australian big-headed ant soldiers were three times more massive than their equivalents in Hawaii, Wills said. The ants at the other study sites were intermediate in size.

Genetic analysis showed that the ants from all the study sites were closely related, Suarez said, ruling out the possibility that size variation was the result of long-term evolutionary change.

"This shows that they were able to adapt to a new environment relatively quickly," Suarez said. "It's kind of exciting and scary that it can happen so quickly."

(In a separate study, Wills found that fire ants can begin to morph their body size within 60 days in a new environment.)

The big-headed ants produced bigger workers in response to external cues, but they did not generate a larger share of soldiers in any of the sites, Suarez said.

"We found that the ratios of soldiers to non-soldiers were relatively constant across our populations," he said

The researchers hypothesize that the ants use chemical signals called pheromones to determine when the nest has the right proportion of soldiers to other workers. If this turns out to be the case, scientists may be able to control the ants with chemical signals without disrupting the surrounding ecology, Suarez said.


相关文章

  • 2生物入侵的适应性进化及其影响[兼容模式]
  • 生物入侵的适应性进化及其影响 宣劲松 北京科技大学生物科学与工程系 1 外来物种入侵成功要经历存活.定居.繁衍等几个阶段.在这个过程中有些外来种成功地适应了新生态环境变为归化种,而有些则由于不明原因而大规模地爆发.则由于不明原因而大规模地爆 ...查看


  • 生物八上复习提纲
  • 八上复习提纲 2013版 5单元 生物的多样性 第14章 丰富多彩的生物世界 一.生物的类别 植物与人类生活的关系:人类的衣食住行都需要植物(植物吸收二氧化碳释放氧气,合成有机物) 我国的珍稀植物:一级植物(桫椤.水杉.珙桐.金花茶).二级 ...查看


  • 充分理解涌现性
  • 充分理解涌现性,慎重对待转基因 该文发表于<科学家>12月刊(总第2期),pp88-89] 尽管涌现性的概念并不算复杂,但我们还是先从几个例子开始解释. 1. 从三个例子到涌现概念 蚁群的组织性.我们知道,单只蚂蚁的能力是相当有 ...查看


  • 当代科学技术新知识读本(答案)
  • <当代科学技术新知识读本>日照市试卷答案 一.单选题(单选题) 共 20 题 1.1998年,<中国生物多样性国情研究报告>估计,中国生物多样性每年可以提供的服务,折合成经济 价值大概在()万亿美元,(本题分数:2 ...查看


  • 蜜蜂的神奇世界
  • 内容简介 你相信吗?"如果蜜蜂从地球上消失,人类将最多存活四年. 本书的原版(德语版)出版一年销售即超过13000册. 本书截至目前已被译为10种语言(英语.意大利语.希腊语.波兰语.法语.西班牙语.葡萄牙语.韩语.斯洛文尼亚语. ...查看


  • 单元卷13+生态系统和环境保护
  • 单元卷13 生态系统和环境保护 (时间:15分钟 分数:50分) 1. (2015·新课标卷Ⅱ)下列有关生态系统的叙述,错误的是( ) A. 生态系统的组成成分中含有非生物成分 B. 生态系统相对稳定时无能量的输入和散失 C. 生态系统维持 ...查看


  • "自我毁灭"的生存模式--""草坪效应"
  • 王存兴 于 2006-2-8 23:34:00 发布在 凯迪社区 > 猫眼看人 走向"自我毁灭"的生存模式--""草坪效应" 假如我们有一把大大的剪刀,把森林剪的象草坪一样平整,那么森 ...查看


  • 苏教版最新八年级上册生物复习填空
  • 第十四章 丰富多彩的生物世界 第一节五彩缤纷的植物世界 一. 藻类植物 1. 分类 单细胞藻类:_____,_____. 多细胞藻类:____,____,_____. 2. 特征:①_________ ②__________________ ...查看


  • 生命智力在生命起源与进化过程中的复杂性原理(四)
  • /王红旗 四.生命智力学暨智因进化论的复杂性原理 1.生命智力学暨智因进化论的基本原理 生命智力学是一种从生命智力角度描述.解释生命现象.生物行为的全新的生物学理论.与此同时,生命智力学也是一门研究所有生命智力现象.所有生命智力结构或形式. ...查看


热门内容